.

Friday, March 29, 2019

Handedness and Lateralization

Handedness and handedness Handedness and LateralizationCortical Organisation and Lateralization Of The thinker In Handedness And Dominance According to Annett most people in our f sum uprnity correct handedness as the hand that you use for writing (1970). Researchers define handedness as the hand that performs faster or specifically on physical tests. Paul Broca (1979), elicited that a persons handedness was opposite from that specialised noetic rational cerebral hemisphere (so a set-hand(prenominal) person probably has a left(a)-hemispheric diction forte). However, a majority of left-hemispheric intelligence specialise for run-in abilities. Me real researchers ready attack to this correlation between handedness and creationator lateralisation. The key reason that hand- maven joining is Coperni mountain and is an accepted methodology is that clinicians use handedness as a marker for brain dominance. spoken communication is a distributed cerebral ne devil rk with differences in atomic number 18a involvement that relate to specific row functions (Frith et al., 1991). Vital regions in ne devilrk lateralize to one hemisphere and determine lesion (Ojemann, 1991). In most people this lateralization is to the left. The only consistent information on the divergence of hemispheric control between single(a)ists are aphasias following a jibe or hemispheric in activating by agency in patients with brain lesions (Wada and Rasmussen, 1960). Pertaining to the un tied(p)ness of phrase control on that point is a chance of in operation(p) hemispheric reform (Rasmussen and Milner, 1977). It assumes that variation from left hemisphere diction occasion is relate to a difference like left-handedness. In right-handed subjects there is puzzling correlation of verbal linguistic process and hand mandate, both(prenominal) restrain to a undersize area to the left hemisphere (Mayeux and Kandel, 1991). The actual variant of manner of speaking lateralization in the ecumenic population is practically unknown. Evaluations in a re deferative number of lusty subjects do non re throw because, in the past, no technique was available to determine words lateralization effectively and non-invasively.This escape of information has hampered the assessment of phraseology disturbances. There is an on-going debate on the role of the right hemisphere in convalescence from aphasia later on left hemispheric cuffs (Weiller et al., 1993Go Heiss et al., 1997Go Mimura et al., 1998Go). Particularly, in retrospective military ranks it would be important to know how many patients with left hemispheric strokes and transient disturbance of voice communication can be expected to look at been right hemisphere phrase dominant and to have suffered speech impairment due to former(a), to a greater extent spacious causes like decreased vigilance.Moreover, know peakge concerning the exact incidence of right hemisphere language dictum in fit righthanders would be important for usable neuroimaging studies. Here, due to lack of information, researchers often need to rely on the given that restricting examinations to profound right-handers will control for a accomplishable variance in hemispheric confidence.Recently, a simplified structural imaging technique, running(a) transcranial Doppler-ultrasonography (fTCD) has become available (Aaslid, 1987Go Hartje et al., 1994Go Silvestrini et al., 1994Go Rihs et al., 1995Go). It allows determination of hemispheric potency in individual subjects in an effective, reliable and non-invasive direction (Deppe et al., 1997Go Knecht et al., 1998). This technique has now made it possible to establish the discrepancy in the side and degree of language effectiveness in a representative number of healthy subjects. fTCD measures cerebral perfusion changes link up to neuronal activating in a way comparable to working(a) MRI ( practicable magnetic resonance imaging) and 15O- d eary (Kuschinsky, 1991Go Jueptner and Weiller, 1995Go Deppe et al., 1997Go, 1998Go). fTCD makes it possible to compare perfusion changes (by measuring blood execute velocities) inside the territories of the two middle cerebral arteries (MCAs), which present the potential language areas (van der Zwan and Hillen, 1991Go). It thus digests an operational power of laterality which, in many respects, resembles the one obtained by the intracarotid amobarbital procedure (Wada test) (Wada and Rasmussen, 1960Go). decisiveness of language lateralization by fTCD matches precisely both the get outs of fMRI and the Wada test with concordance in e rattling single teddy (Deppe et al., 1998Go Knecht et al., 1998aGo).As in many previous studies of this kind, word genesis was elect as an activating paradigm because it is one of the most effective measures of language production (Neils-Strunjas, 1998Go). On this basis language confidence was determined in a total of 188 healthy subjects. Lef t-handers were excluded from the bring because of possible confounding make of handedness on hemispheric dominance (Kimura, 1983Go). A careful tarradiddle for brain damage in the prenatal period or in infancy was taken in order to exclude subjects with possible ductile reorganization of hemispheric dominance later brain lesions (Rasmussen and Milner, 1977Go).The work was pull up stakes of the Munster practicable imaging study on the variability of hemispheric specialization in health and disease (Deppe et al., 1997Go Knecht et al., 1998aGo, bGo). Hemispheric language dominance was assessed in 188 healthy volunteers with 111 fe potents (mean age 26 5.5 years, range 17-50 years) and 77 males (mean age 27 3.7 years, range 21-40 years). Subjects were excluded if, on a standardized questionnaire, they reported delayed or disturbed language development or a history of other neurological disorders, pickyly perinatal asphyxia or kernicterus, take trauma, way out of consciousness , epileptic seizures, meningitis or encephalitis. They were further required to have successfully spotless the equivalent of high school (Realschule or Gymnasium). sound-handedness was assessed by a handedness indicant in the Edinburgh Inventory of greater than 30% (Oldfield, 1971Go). Left-handers were excluded from the study, as were right-handers with a score for right-handedness lower than 30%, because, due to the small number of these subjects, an adequate evaluation of the effect of handedness on language lateralization would not have been possible. Approximately 75% of the subjects recruited had an index of to a greater extent than 80% right-handedness. in all subjects gave informed consent to participate in this study, which was O.K. by the Ethics Committee of the University of Mnster.Assessment of hemispheric language dominance was performed by a standardized fTCD technique (used in a number of previous studies) and a word times projection, validated by direct analog y with the intracarotid amobarbital injection and fMRI (Knecht et al., 1996Go, 1997Go, 1998aGo, bGo Deppe et al., 1997Go, 1998Go). Briefly, subjects were presented with a letter on a computer screen 2.5 s after a cueing shade. silently they had to find as many actors line as possible scratch with the displayed letter. For fTCD an activation paradigm strongly based on verbal suaveness was used, corresponding to the fields of reported female superiority (Basso et al., 1982Go Pizzamiglio et al., 1985Go). Task proceeding was control direct by instructing the subjects to report the words after a second auditory signal following 15 s after presentation of the letter. All words had to be reported within a 5-s time period. The abutting letter was presented in the same way after a quiet period of 60 s. Letters were presented in random order and no letter was displayed more than once. Q, X and Y were excluded because very few words have these as initial letter.Changes in the cerebral blood proceed velocity (CBFV) in the basal arteries were measured as an indicator of the downstream increase of the regional metabolic activity during the language task. Dual fTCD of the MCAs was performed with two 2 MHz transducer probes attached to a headband and placed symmetrically at the temporal skull windows (1Go). Details of the insonation technique, particularly the correct identification of the MCA, have been make elsewhere (Ringelstein et al., 1990Go). The spectral envelope curves of the Doppler signal were analysed off-line with the fTCD software mediocre developed by one of the authors (M.D.) (Deppe et al., 1997Go).1 Schematic diagram of the way language lateralization was determined. Perfusion increases and therefore neuronal activation during word generation were assessed in the vascular territories of the left (marked in red) and right (marked in green) MCAs, which comprise the language areas. This was achieved by fTCD steps of the CBFV changes in these arterie s. Systemic set up were eliminated by reason the differences in perfusion changes between sides. Averaging the responses over 20 repetitions (on average) in each individual made the results highly reliable. (For details, see Deppe et al., 1997.)After automated artefact rejection, entropy were integrated over the corresponding cardiac cycles, segmented into epochs which related to the cueing tone and then averaged. The epochs were set to vex 15 s before and to fire 35 s after the cueing tone. The mean velocity in the 15-s pre-cueing time interval (Vpre.mean) was taken as the base-line value. The sexual relation CBFV changes (dV) during cerebral activation were calculated apply the formula dV = V(t) Vpre.mean x 100 / Vpre.mean where V(t) is the CBFV over time. Relative CBFV changes from repeated presentations of letters (on average 20 runs) were averaged time-locked to the cueing tone. The number of repetitions was less than 22, because no letter was presented more than once d uring the word generation task.A functional TCD laterality index LIfTCDwas calculated using the formulaStatisticsThe Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was used to assess the dead reckoning that laterality indices in males and females were drawn from different populations. Unlike the parametric t-test for indie samples or the Mann-Whitney U test, which tests for differences in the location of two samples (differences in means, differences in average ranks, respectively), the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test is sensitive to differences in the general shapes of the distributions in the two samples, i.e. to differences in dispersion and skewness (Spence et al., 1990Go). The Mann-Whitney test for equivalence (Wellek, 1996Go) was employed to verify equivalence of laterality indices in men and women. A significant result in this test provides a strong positive measure for a lack of gender differences in laterality indices. We tested the null possibleness H0 PLImale LIfemale 1/2 =varepsilonversus the a lternative hypothesis of equivalence H1 PLImale LIfemale 1/2 In six of the 194 right-handed subjects determination of language lateralization was not possible due to lack of a temporal bone window, i.e. unequal ultrasonographic penetration of the skull by the ultrasound beam. In the remaining 188 subjects (59% females, 41% males) the boilersuit distribution of language lateralization was bimodal with 7.5% being right hemisphere and 92.5% left hemisphere language dominant (2Go).The distribution of language lateralization was equivalent in men and women (3Go). The Kolmogorov-Smirnov test did not detect any significant differences between females and males in the overall distribution (P 0.05). In the subgroup of left hemisphere language dominant subjects, the Mann-Whitney test for equivalence furnished equivalence with P The average number of words found during the activation task per letter presented was not statistically different between men and women (Mann-Whitney U test, P = 0.81) or subjects with left or right hemisphere language dominance (Mann-Whitney U test, P = 0.26). It was also independent of the index of lateralization (correlation coefficient r = 0.027).These are the first information on the natural distribution of language dominance in a sizable serial publication of healthy right-handed subjects. They demonstrate equivalence of language lateralization for word generation in males and females, and they suggest that 1 in 13 healthy right-handed subjects is right hemisphere dominant for language.MethodologyThere is debate whether language can be treated as a separate mental competency or should be approached as part of a more general cognitive system (Fodor, 1983Go). Moreover, language comprises receptive and expressive aspects and is intertwined with prosody, memory and vigilance (Knecht et al., 1996Go Binder et al., 1997Go). Therefore, the assessment of language lateralization based on a single activation task provides just one index of t he interindividual variability in language processing. This approach can nevertheless serve as a first step in elucidating the factors underlying the diversity of large scale neural language organization.fTCD lends itself to determination of hemispheric language dominance. The index of lateralization obtained by fTCD based on word generation is very reliable and closely corresponds to (i) the outcome of the intracarotid amobarbital procedure and (ii) the index of lateralization obtained by fMRI (Deppe et al., 1998Go Knecht et al., 1998aGo). Other techniques like head turning, offspring-related potentials, transcranial high frequency magnetised stimulation or the dichotic listening test used for the evaluation of language dominance have so far failed to provide results that are coherent and in sufficient concordance with the intracarotid amobarbital procedure (Bryden and Allard, 1981Go Jancke et al., 1992Go Jennum et al., 1994Go Segalowitz and Berge, 1995Go OLeary et al., 1996Go H ugdahl et al., 1997Go).Unlike the intracarotid amobarbital procedure and as opposed to brain lesions, functional imaging techniques including fTCD assess brain activation and not inactivation. They are set to determine the location and relative add up of the maximal activation while diffuse or symmetrical activations are cancelled out. Thus, fTCD is insensitive to a lesser activation in the contralateral hemisphere. Moreover, fTCD cannot determine whether an activated region during a task is a comminuted region that, when damaged, will result in a loss of that particular function. This shortcoming holds for all functional imaging techniques. However, the fact that determination of language lateralization by fMRI and fTCD correspond closely to that determined by the intracarotid amobarbital inactivation suggests that activated regions match critical regions and therefore provide essential information on the risk for language loss (Desmond et al., 1995Go Binder et al., 1996Go Knech t et al., 1998aGo). finishFuelled by the general interest in la petite diffrence, the lack of information nigh the natural distribution of language dominance has led to far-reaching speculations about possible differences in language lateralizations between the sexes. This discussion has been characterized by a high acceptance for positive results. Thus, despite considerable data to the contrary, there is a strong belief that language in women, on average, is less lateralized than in men (Bakan and Putnam, 1974Go charge and Reid, 1976Go McGlone, 1980Go McKeever et al., 1983Go Hough et al., 1994Go Rugg, 1995Go). The idea of an increased bilaterality in women has received support by a new-fashioned fMRI study in 19 males and 19 females (Shaywitz et al., 1995Go) in which activation related to a rhyming task was found to be more bilateral in women than in men. It has been conjectured that an increased bilaterality of language in women would lead to a decreased susceptibility to unila teral infarctions explaining a greater male than female proportion of aphasics (McGlone, 1980Go).Kertesz and Sheppard then showed that aphasias were as frequent in males as in females, as long as sex differences in the incidence of infarcts were taken into account (Kertesz and Sheppard, 1981Go). Similar results were obtained in a more recent epidemiological study (Pedersen et al., 1995Go). Recently, using fMRI, Frost and colleagues found no differences between sexes during a language comprehension task when group averages were compared (Frost et al., 1999Go). Our data provide the first direct evidence that language lateralization during word generation in men and women is also equivalent in variablity. In fact, they not only show a lack of significant differences but they positively demonstrate significance of equivalence in healthy subjects even though this finding is based on a word generation task, i.e. a field of reported female superiority (Kimura and Harshman, 1984Go). Equival ence of hemispheric lateralization between sexes during word generation does not exclude gender differences in subfunctions of language like rhyming, which we did not investigate. As was pointed out before, such a difference has been reported by Shaywitz and colleagues in a small series of subjects examined by fMRI (Shaywitz et al., 1995Go). However, in line with our results, these researchers did not find gender differences in other language tasks.Right hemisphere language dominanceThe predominance of right-handedness and left hemisphere language lateralization has led some theorists to suggest that a gestural system of communication with dominance of the right hand provided the neural architecture for vocal articulation in merciful evolution (Hewes, 1973Go Kimura, 1987Go). If indeed handedness and language were coupled because they treat the same neural resources, then any deviation from this pattern would have to be pathologic. Right hemisphere language dominance in right-han ders or left hemisphere language dominance in left-handers reported from the intracarotid amobarbital procedure does not challenge this view, because this procedure is only performed in patients with brain pathology. However, the present findings in healthy subjects indicate that even under natural conditions the familiarity between handedness and language dominance is not an absolute one. Because 75% of subjects were strongly right-handed (80%) and the remaining had handedness indices of 30%, the effect of the degree of handedness on language lateralization could not be evaluated in the present study. Comparison of left- and right-handers will be necessary to test whether a relative association between handedness and language dominance exists in healthy subjects.The extreme argument could be put forward that all of our presumed healthy subjects with right hemisphere dominance must have suffered covert brain damage resulting in a shift of language into the right hemisphere. A conn atural argument has been made to explain left-handedness in healthy subjects (Coren, 1990Go). We believe that covert brain damage was unlikely. The medical history in all subjects was unrevealing and the scholastic achievement was similar. The average number of words produced during the task did not differ between subjects with left or right hemisphere language dominance and the pattern of language lateralization variability was bimodal with maxima for left- and right-hemisphere dominance (2Go). If there had been subclinical damage to language relevant areas in the left hemisphere resulting in a shift to the right, one would have expected impaired word fluency and more cases with little lateralization because of a bilateral representation of language functions. This was not the case. We therefore suggest that right hemisphere language dominance is not a pathological but a natural phenomenon.Previous estimates of atypical right hemisphere language dominance were either based on the r esults from the intracarotid amobarbital test in patients evaluated for resective neurosurgery or on the occurrence of crossed aphasia, i.e. aphasias after right hemispheric lesions. In patients with epilepsy submitted to the intracarotid amobarbital test the number of right-handers with right hemisphere language dominance was 4% in a large series and rose to 12% when a left hemisphere lesion was defined (Rasmussen and Milner, 1977Go). Because the Wada test is only performed in patients with brain lesions, which are often associated with a secondary transfer of cortical functions from the damaged to the whole hemisphere, these numbers cannot be extrapolated to healthy subjects (Helmstaedter et al., 1994Go). By evaluation of stroke-patients with crossed aphasia, the incidence of right hemisphere language dominance in right-handers has been inferred to be between 1 and 2% in the majority of series (Gloning, 1977Go Borod et al., 1985Go Kertesz, 1985Go).On the one hand, this low estima te of right hemisphere language dominance in previously healthy subjects made aphasias in right-handers after right-sided lesions seem an exceptional event and has resulted in almost 100 reports on crossed aphasia in the last 30 years. On the other hand, difficulties in the assessment of language performance due to physical exhaustion and deficits in sustained attention in the early stages after stroke and reorganizational reappearance in the later stages may have facilitated an underdiagnosis of aphasia in right hemispheric stroke patients in many studies. Not every patient with a cerebral infarction in the respective language dominant hemisphere will suffer damage of the language areas and become aphasic. The overall rate of aphasia due to stroke has been found to be 38% in the lancinate state and 18% at discharge from the hospital (Pedersen et al., 1995Go). Reasoning from the make of brain activation to the effects of brain lesions is problematic but results from activation s tudies may be conceptually useful to the understanding of lesion-deficit variability in the clinical context (Willmes and Poeck, 1993Go). In a single recent study on 880 stroke patients it was reported, in passing, that of right-handed aphasics 9% had right hemispheric lesions (Pedersen et al., 1995Go). In a study on language deficits in servicemen who had suffered abrupt brain wounds, 18% of the aphasics had suffered right hemispheric lesions (Mohr et al., 1980Go). However, here the possible effects of diffuse brain damage by the impact of a locoweed and the effect of variable handedness pose methodological limitations. Our cohort was similar in age to these soldiers. We found an incidence of 7.5% of right hemisphere dominance in our activation study of healthy subjects. This combined evidence suggests that about 1 in 13 previously healthy right-handed patients with a right hemispheric infarction could be at risk of scathe language impairments because this is the hemisphere domi nant for word generation. Conversely, after left hemispheric infarctions right-handed patients, who in retrospective evaluations seem to have recovered vigorous from language disturbances, and on fMRI or PET may even show language related activation in the right hemisphere, may do so because they had been right hemisphere language dominant to begin with.Presently, we do not know the relevance of the extent of language lateralization by fTCD. Low indices of lateralization indicate that there is a bihemispheric activation during word generation. Although reported in studies based on the Wada test, bilateral language representation in stroke patients has probably been neglected because persistent aphasia in these subjects may only occur after bilateral damage (Benbadis et al., 1995Go). This is very rare and patients rarely survive. However, subjects with low indices of lateralization may be the ones who, after unilateral damage of traditional language regions, do not show marked aphas ia and recover well by further recruitment of the intact hemisphere.Aaslid R. Visually evoked dynamic blood extend response of the human cerebral circulation. Stroke 1987 18 771-5.Abstract/FreeFullTextBakan P, Putnam W. Right-left discrimination and brain lateralization. sex activity differences. soaked Neurol 1974 30 334-5.Abstract/FreeFullTextBasso A, Capitani E, Moraschini S. Sex differences in recovery from aphasia. Cortex 1982 18 469-75.Web of ScienceMedlineBenbadis SR, Dinner DS, Chelune GJ, Piedmonte M, Lders HO. Autonomous versus dependent a classification of bilateral language representation by intracarotid amobarbital procedure. J Epilepsy 1995 8 255-63.Web of ScienceBinder JR, Swanson SJ, Hammeke TA, Morris GL, Mueller WM, Fischer M, et al. Determination of language dominance using functional MRI a comparison with the Wada test. clinical neurology 1996 46 978-84.Abstract/FreeFullTextBinder JR, Frost JA, Hammeke TA, Cox RW, Rao SM, Prieto T. Human brain language areas i dentified by functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci 1997 17 353-62.Abstract/FreeFullTextBorod JC, Carper M, Naeser M, Goodglass H. Left-handed and right-handed aphasics with left hemisphere lesions compared on nonverbal performance measures. Cortex 1985 21 81-90.Web of ScienceMedlineBryden MD, Allard FA. Do auditory perceptual asymmetries develop? Cortex 1981 17 313-8.Web of ScienceMedlineCoren S. Left-handedness in offspring as a function of maternal age at parturition letter. N Engl J Med 1990 322 1673.Web of ScienceMedlineDeppe M, Knecht S, Henningsen H, Ringelstein E-B. AVERAGE a Windows program for automated analysis of event related cerebral blood fertilise. J Neurosci Methods 1997 75 147-54.Web of ScienceMedlineDeppe M, Knecht S, Papke K, Fleischer H, Ringelstein EB, Henningsen H. Correlation of cerebral blood flow velocity and regional cerebral blood flow during word generation. Neuroimage 1998 7(4 Pt 2) S448.Desmond JE, Sum JM, Wagner AD, Demb JB, Shear PK, Glov er GH, et al. Functional MRI measurement of language lateralization in Wada-tested patients. brain 1995 118 1411-9.Abstract/FreeFullTextFodor JA. The modularity of mind. Cambridge (MA) MIT Press 1983.Frith CD, Friston KJ, Liddle PF, Frackowiak RS. A PET study of word finding. Neuropsychologia 1991 29 1137-48.Web of ScienceMedlineFrost JA, Binder JR, Springer JA, Hammeke TA, Bellgowan PS, Rao SM, et al. Language processing is strongly left lateralized in both sexes. Evidence from functional MRI. Brain 1999 122 199-208.Abstract/FreeFullTextGloning K. Handedness and aphasia. Neuropsychologia 1977 15 355-8.Web of ScienceMedlineHartje W, Ringelstein EB, Kistinger B, Fabianek D, Willmes K. Transcranial Doppler ultrasonic assessment of middle cerebral artery blood flow velocity changes during verbal and visuospatial cognitive tasks. Neuropsychologia 1994 32 1443-52.Web of ScienceMedlineHeiss WD, Karbe H, Weber-Luxenburger G, Herholz K, Kessler J, Pietrzyk U, et al. Speech-induced cerebral metabolic activation reflects recovery from aphasia. J Neurol Sci 1997 145 213-7.Web of ScienceMedlineHelmstaedter C, Kurthen M, Linke DB, Elger CE. Right hemisphere restitution of language and memory functions in right hemisphere language-dominant patients with left temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain 1994 117 729-37.Abstract/FreeFullTextHewes GW. Primate communication and the gestural origin of language. Curr Anthropol 1973 14 5-32.Hough MS, Daniel HJ, Snow MA, OBrien KF, Hume WG. Gender differences in laterality patterns for speaking and singing. Neuropsychologia 1994 32 1067-78.Web of ScienceMedlineHugdahl K, Carlsson G, Uvebrant P, Lundervold AJ. Dichotic-listening performance and intracarotid injections of amobarbital in children and adolescents. Arch Neurol 1997 54 1494-500.Abstract/FreeFullTextJancke L, Steinmetz H, Volkmann J. Dichotic listening what does it measure? Neuropsychologia 1992 30 941-50.Web of ScienceMedlineJennum P, Friberg L, Fuglsang-Frederiksen A, Dam M. Speech l ocalization using repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurology 1994 44 269-73.Abstract/FreeFullTextJueptner M, Weiller C. Review does measurement of regional cerebral blood flow reflect synaptic activity? Implications for PET and fMRI. Neuroimage 1995 2 148-56.Web of ScienceMedlineKertesz A. Aphasia. In Vinken PJ, Bruyn GW, Klawans HL, editors. vade mecum of clinical neurology, Vol. 45. Amsterdam Elsevier 1985. p. 287-331.Kertesz A, Sheppard A. The epidemiology of aphasic and cognitive impairment in stroke age, sex, aphasia type and laterality differences. Brain 1981 104 117-28.FreeFullTextKimura D. Speech representation in an indifferent sample of left-handers. Hum Neurobiol 1983 2 147-54.MedlineKimura D. The origin of human communication. In Robson J, editor. starting time and evolution of the universe evidence for design? Montreal McGill-Queens Press 1987. p. 227-46.Kimura D, Harshman RA. Sex differences in brain organization for verbal and non-verbal functions. Re view. Prog Brain Res 1984 61 423-41.Web of ScienceMedlineKnecht S, Henningsen H, Deppe M, Huber T, Ebner A, Ringelstein E-B. accompanying activation of both cerebral hemispheres during cued word generation. Neuroreport 1996 7 820-4.Web of ScienceMedlineKnecht S, Deppe M, Bcker M, Ringelstein E-B, Henningsen H. regional cerebral blood flow increases during preparation for and processing of sensory stimuli. Exp Brain Res 1997 116 309-14.Web of ScienceMedlineKnecht S, Deppe M, Ebner A, Henningsen H, Huber T, Jokeit H, et al. Noninvasive determination of language lateralization by functional transcranial Doppler sonography a comparison with the Wada test. Stroke 1998a 29 82-6.Abstract/FreeFullTextKnecht S, Deppe M, Ringelstein E-B, Wirtz M, Lohmann H, Drger B, et al. Reproducibility of functional transcranial Doppler sonography in determining hemispheric language lateralization. Stroke 1998b 29 1155-9.Abstract/FreeFullTextKuschinsky W. Coupling of function, metabolism, and blood flow i n the brain. Review. Neurosurg Rev 1991 14 163-8.Web of ScienceMedlineLevy J, Reid M. Variations in writing posture and cerebral organization. Science 1976 194 337-9.Abstract/FreeFullTextMayeux R, Kandel ER. Disorders of language the aphasias. In Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessell TM, editors. Principle of neural science. 3rd ed. London Prentice Hall 1991. p. 839-51.McGlone J. Sex differences in human brain asymmetry a critical survey. Behav Brain Sci 1980 5 215-64.McKeever WF, Seitz KS, Hoff AL, Marino MF, Diehl JA. Interacting sex and familial sinistrality characteristics influence both language lateralization and spatial ability in right handers. Neuropsychologia 1983 21 661-8.Web of ScienceMedlineMimura M, Kato M, Sano Y, Kojima T, Naeser M, Kashima H. likely and retrospective studies of recovery in aphasia. Changes in cerebral blood flow and language functions. Brain 1998 121 2083-94.Abstract/FreeFullTextMohr JP, Weiss GH, Caveness WF, Dillon JD, Kistler JP, Meirowsky AM, et al. Language and motor disorders after penetrating head injury in Vietnam. Neurology 1980 30 1273-9.Abstract/FreeFullTextNeil

No comments:

Post a Comment